Baboons shed light on antimicrobial resistance. Tasks
Поможем в ✍️ написании учебной работы
Поможем с курсовой, контрольной, дипломной, рефератом, отчетом по практике, научно-исследовательской и любой другой работой

Task1. Find words/expressions meaning the following.

1.  change

2.  important or significant;

3.  influence, effect;

4.  way in , approach;

5.  to associate (a group of elements or qualities) with an equivalent group;

6.  to be more concerned with one thing than with other things;

7.  made, done, or happening without method or conscious decision;

8.  carefully;

9.  diminished in number or quality;

Task2. Say whether the following is true- , false;. or not mentioned.

1.  All baboons have the same microbes in their intestines.

2.  The composition of the human gut microbiome depends on people’ s way of life.

3.  Since baboons have an inborn antibiotic resistance , it is in no way affected by theircontact with people.

4.  In wild baboons, social group membership predicts the identity and abundance of gut microbes, while kinship, age, and sex do not.

5.  The researchers found that the microbe compositions of wild baboons and that of some African tribe are very much alike.

6.  The variation in baboons microbe composition could be due to the fact that baboons from the same social group tend to be more closely related and eat the same food .

7.  The researchers were surprised to find resistance genes in baboons that had never contacted with humans,

8.  The wild baboons are believed to have inherited antibiotic resistance from their ancestors and it has nothing to do with human influence.

9.  It is not yet clear why the bacteria disappearing in humans with age are abundant even in the adult baboon population.

Task3. Answer the following questions.

1.  How does the gut microbe composition of baboons differ from population to population?

2.  What are the differences in the human gut microbiome due to?

3.  What methods did the researchers use to find antiibiotic resistance genes in baboons?

4.  What similarities did they find between humans and baboons?

5.  How can antibiotic resistance genes in baboons which have had no contact with people be explained?

6.  If there were no antibiotics where does antibiotic resistance come from?

7.  Why were the researchers surprised?

8.  What is so unusual about these bacteria?

9.  How can this phenomenon be explained?


YOUR BRAIN IS SPECIAL.

So is mine. Differences arise at every level of the organ’s astonishingly intricate architecture; the human brain contains 100 billion neurons, which come in thousands of types and collectively form an estimate of more than 100 trillion interconnections. These differences, in turn, lead to variances in the ways we think, learn and behave and in our propensity for mental illness.

How does diversity in brain wiring and function arise? Variations in the genes we inherit from our parents can play a role. Yet even identical twins raised by the same parents can differ markedly in their mental functioning, behavioral traits, and risk of mental illness or neurodegenerative disease. In fact, mice bred to be genetically identical that are then handled in exactly the same way in the laboratory display differences in learning ability, fear avoidance and responses to stress even when age, gender and care are held constant. Something more has to be going on.

Certainly the experiences we have in life matter as well; they can, for instance, influence the strength of the connections between particular sets of neurons. But researchers are increasingly finding tantalizing indications that other factors are at work—for instance, processes that mutate genes or affect gene behavior early in an embryo’s development or later in life. Such phenomena include alternative splicing, in which a single gene can give rise to two or more different proteins. Proteins carry out most of the operations in cells, and thus which proteins are made in cells will affect the functioning of the tissues those cells compose. Many researchers are also exploring the role of epigenetic changes—DNA modifications that alter gene activity (increasing or decreasing the synthesis of specific proteins) without changing the information in genes.

In the past few years the two of us and our colleagues have come on especially intriguing suspects that seem to operate more in the brain than in other tissues: jumping genes. Such genes, which have been found in virtually all species, including humans, can paste copies of themselves into other parts of the genome (the full set of DNA in the nucleus) and alter the functioning of the affected cell, making it behave differently from an otherwise identical cell right next to it. Many such insertions in many different cells would be expected to yield subtle or not so subtle differences in cognitive abilities, personality traits and susceptibility to neurological problems.

Our early findings of gene jumping in the brain have led us to another question: Given that the brain’s proper functioning is essential to survival, why has evolution allowed a process that tinkers with its genetic programming to persist? Although we still do not have a definite answer, mounting evidence suggests that by inducing variability in brain cells, jumping genes may imbue organisms with the flexibility to adapt quickly to changing circumstances. Therefore, these jumping genes—or mobile elements, as they are called—may have been retained evolutionarily because, from the standpoint of promoting survival of the species, this adaptation benefit outweighs the risks.

ANCIENT INVADERS

THE IDEA THAT MOBILE ELEMENTS EXIST and move about in the genome is not new, but the recent evidence that they are so active in the brain came as a surprise. Gene jumping was first discovered in plants, even before James Watson and Francis Crick spelled out the double-helical structure of DNA in 1953. In the 1940s Barbara McClintock of Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory observed that “con-trolling elements” moved from one place to another in the genetic material of corn plants. She discovered that under stress, certain regions in the genome could migrate and turn genes on and off in their new location. The products of McClintock’s experiments were the now famous ears of corn with seeds of varying colors—a demonstration of genetic mosaicism, in which genes in a particular cell may be switched on or off in a pattern that differs from that of neighboring cells that are otherwise identical.

McClintock’s research, which at first encountered skepticism within the scientific community, eventually resulted in her receiving a Nobel Prize in 1983. In subsequent years it became clear that the phenomenon of genetic mosaicism is not restricted to plants but also occurs in many organisms, including humans.

McClintock did her work on transposons, which are mobile elements that use a cut-and-paste mechanism to move a stretch of DNA around the cell’s genome. More recent research on mobile elements in the brain had focused on retrotransposons, which employ a copy-and-paste approach to insinuate themselves into new areas of the genome. They basically replicate themselves rather than popping out of the surrounding DNA, after which the copy takes up a new position elsewhere.

Retrotransposons make up as much as half of the nucleotides, or DNA building blocks, in the human genome. In contrast, the approximately 25,000 protein-coding genes we possess make up less than 2 percent of mammalian DNA. The jumping genes are descendants of the first primitive molecular replication systems that invaded the genomes of eukaryotes (organisms having cells that contain a nucleus) long ago. A group led by Haig H. Kazazian, Jr., at the University of Pennsylvania showed in 1988 that retrotransposons, which were once thought of as nonfunctional junk DNA, were active in human tissues.

In particular, one type of retrotransposon, known as a long interspersed element 1 (L1), appears to be a key player in the human genome. It is able to hop around frequently probably because it, unlike other mobile elements in humans, encodes its own machinery for spreading copies of itself far and wide in the cellular genome. Analysis of its behavior in cells reveals that when something prompts an L1 in the nuclear genome to begin the “jumping” process, it first transcribes itself into single-stranded RNA, which then travels from the nucleus to the cytoplasm, where it serves as a template for constructing proteins specified by some parts of the L1 DNA. The proteins then form a molecular complex with the still intact RNA, and the whole complex heads back to the nucleus. There one of the proteins, an enzyme called an endonuclease, makes a nick in specific sites in the DNA. It also uses the RNA as a template for producing a double-stranded DNA copy of the original L1 retrotransposon and inserts this duplicate into the genome where the cut was made. Such reverse transcription, from RNA to DNA, is familiar to many people today as part of the way that the HIV virus gets a DNA copy of its RNA genome to take up a permanent home in the genome of the cells it infects.

Retrotransposition often fails to run its course, which produces truncated, nonfunctional copies of the original L1 DNA. Sometimes these snippets (or the whole L1 copy) have no effect on a protein-coding gene. Other times, though, they can have any of several consequences, both good and bad, for a cell’s fate. They may, for instance, drop into and thus alter the protein-coding region of a gene. This maneuver can lead to creation of a new variant of the protein that helps or harms an organism. Or this positioning may stop a given protein from being made. In other instances, the newly pasted DNA may fall outside of a coding region but act as a promoter (a switch that can turn on nearby genes) and alter the level of gene expression—the amount of protein made from the gene—with, once again, good or bad results for the cell and the organism. When LI retrotransposons end up in many places in neurons or in many cells of the brain, or both, the brain will be very different from the one that would have formed without their influence. It stands to reason that such genetic mosaicism could affect behavior, cognition and disease risk and could also help explain why one identical twin may remain disease-free when a sibling is diagnosed with schizophrenia, for example.

WHERE DOES JUMPING OCCUR?

UNTIL RECENTLY, most investigators aware of L1 retrotransposi tion assumed that it mostly took place in germ cells (ovaries or testes). Although a few clues suggested that L1 genes stay active in somatic tissues (nonsex cells) during early development or later, these clues were generally dismissed. If genes exist merely to propagate themselves, as one evolutionary theory holds, jumping genes would have little cause to remain active in somatic cells because such cells would not pass the DNA to an organism’s next generation: after all, the affected cells die when their owner does.

Better detection tools have now revealed that retrotransposons can move around somatic tissues during early development and even later in life. These events happen more often in the brain than in other tissues—a direct challenge to the longstanding dogma that the genetic codes of brain cells in adults are identical to one another and remain stable for the cells’ life.

In our lab at the Salk Institute for Biological Studies in La Jolla, Calif., for instance, we monitored gene jumping in a mouse whose cells were genetically engineered to undergo retrotransposition and fluoresce green when an L1 element inserted itself in genomes of a cell anywhere in its body. We observed glowing green cells only in germ cells and in certain brain areas, including the hippocampus (a region important to memory and attention)— which suggests that L1s may move around more in the brain than in other somatic tissues. Interestingly, the jumping was occurring in progenitor cells that give rise to hippocampal neurons.

In various organs of fully formed organisms, a small population of progenitor cells stands by, ready to divide and give rise to specialized cell types needed to replace cells that die. The hippocampus is one of two regions of the brain where neurogenesis, generation of new nerve cells, occurs. Thus, L1s appear to be active during early development when neurons are being born, but they can also move around in the adult brain in the areas where new neurons continue to be born into adulthood.

Even with the mouse experiments, more evidence was needed that retrotransposition was actually occurring in the brain. We undertook an analysis of human postmortem material that compared the number of L1 elements in brain, heart and liver tissues. We found that the brain tissue contained significantly more L1 elements in each cell nucleus than the heart or liver tissues did.

Much of the jumping had to have occurred during the brain’s development because retrotransposition requires cell division— a process that does not take place in the brain, except in two circumscribed areas—to happen after early childhood. An analysis suggested that each neural cell in humans undergoes an average of 80 L1 integration events, a rate that could well lead to a great deal of variation among cells and in the overall brain activity of different individuals.

A recent finding from researchers at the Roslin Institute near Edinburgh and their colleagues supplies further confirmation of L1 activity in the human brain. The researchers reported in 2011 in Nature that a total of 7,743 insertions of L1s in the hippocampus and caudate nucleus (which is also involved in memory) in three deceased individuals contained integrated L1 elements. (Scientific American is part of Nature Publishing Group.) That study also implied that the emerging portrait of genetic diversity in the brain will only get more complicated as this research moves forward. The Roslin team was surprised to come on about 15,000 members of a class of retrotransposons known as short interspersed elements (SINEs). The preponderant SINE, part of a group known as Alu elements, had never been encountered before in the brain.

Our findings made us wonder what might trigger L1 activity. Knowing that the hippocampus is also a site where neurogenesis transpires and that exposure to novel situations and exercise triggers neurogenesis in mice, we decided to see if exercise might be one spur to gene jumping. We found that after our transgenic mice ran on a wheel, the number of green fluorescing cells increased about twofold in the rodents’ hippocampus. Given that novelty and challenge also prompt neurogenesis, we are entertaining the possibility that a new or unfamiliar environment could be another instigator of retrotransposition.

If we are correct and L1 jumping does increase as the nervous system learns and adapts to the outside world, the finding would indicate that individual brains and the neuronal networks that make them up are constantly changing and alter with each new experience, even in genetically identical twins.

ORIGINS OF DISEASE

WE ARE CONTINUING TO EXPAND the evidence for the hypothesis that jumping genes contribute to human variation in brain processing by moving beyond just counting L1s in DNA. In our quest to link our data to real events that have either positive or detrimental effects on living people, it is sometimes easiest to pinpoint the bad outcomes that resulted from a gene that jumped, if only because the consequences are so obvious.

In November 2010 our team reported in Nature that a mutation in a gene called MeCP2 affected L1 retrotransposition in the brain. Mutations in the MeCP2 gene can induce Rett syndrome, a severe disorder of brain development that almost exclusively affects girls. The discovery that MeCP2 was mutated in patients with Rett syndrome and other mental disorders raised multiple questions about the molecular and cellular mechanisms of this disease. Our research showed that the mutation in the brains of mice and humans with Rett syndrome resulted in a significant increase in numbers of L1 insertions in their neurons—a finding that suggests that the jumping genes might account for some of the effects of the MeCP2 mutation.

L1 activity has also turned up in other disorders. An analysis of the frontal cortex regions of individuals with schizophrenia revealed increased production of mobile element sequences compared with those without the condition. Circumstantial evidence suggests that L1 elements are an important component of various brain disorders, including autism. Understanding the role of mobile elements in the development of psychiatric diseases might lead to new methods for diagnosis, treatment and prevention.

The continuing research into jumping genes in the brain could potentially challenge an entire academic discipline. Behavioral geneticists often follow groups of identical twins over long periods to control for the effects of genes and determine the environmental contributions to such disorders as schizophrenia. The new findings showing that jumping genes actively revise genomes after an embryo forms question the assumption that “identical” twins are genetically alike. Indeed, the new discoveries will make it ever harder to disentangle the relative effects of nature and nurture on our psyches.

The question remains: Why has evolution not destroyed these vestiges of ancient viruses from within our cells, given that jumping genes have a high chance of introducing potentially fatal genetic flaws? To answer the question, we should acknowledge that humans have always been under attack by viral parasites and other invaders that expand the size of our genomes with jumping DNA. The bodies of humans and our evolutionary forebears may not have been able to fully eliminate the interlopers, but they have adapted to at least coexist with the invaders by silencing them through a variety of clever mechanisms that mutate and disable them. It also appears that, in some cases, our genomes have commandeered the genetic machinery of L1 retroelements to enhance our own survival, which is one reason that cells may sometimes allow, or even encourage, L1s to jump around the genome under carefully controlled conditions.

One clue to why they persist may come from closer analysis of the finding that mice from a single genetic strain raised under highly controlled conditions vary greatly in their responses to stress. The observed behavioral differences are distributed typically in the population (picture a bell curve), a pattern that implies that the mechanisms producing this variability are random, as the sites of L1 retrotransposon insertions seem to be.

The putatively random nature of how L1s move from place to place in the genome implies that natural selection may, in effect, be rolling the dice in the hope that benefits from helpful insertions will outweigh any deleterious consequences of other insertions. And nature may be placing bets frequently on the neural progenitor cells of the hippocampus so as to maximize the possibility that at least some of the new positions will give rise to a population of adult neurons particularly well suited to the tasks the brain will confront. A somewhat similar process occurs when the DNA in immune cells rearranges itself to produce an array of antibodies, after which only the antibodies best equipped to fight off a pathogen are selected for full-scale production.

This scenario does not seem far-fetched. L1-mediated effects do not need to be large and do not have to occur in many cells to influence behavior. In rodents, a change in the firing pattern of a single neuron might be enough to make a difference.

More possible support for this idea is the discovery that the only lineage of L1 jumping elements currently active in the human genome evolved about 2.7 million years ago, after the evolutionary split from chimpanzees to bipedal humans—a time when our hominid ancestors were first beginning to adopt the use of stone tools. That finding lends credence to the notion that the L1 elements may have helped build brains that can process information about the environment rapidly and that can thus more readily meet the challenges of ever changing environmental and climatic conditions. L1 jumping genes seem to have been a collaborative partner in advancing the evolution of Homo sapiens.  

Your brain is special, tasks.

Part1, p.1-2(up to ‘Retrotransposition often fails…’)

Task1. Find words/ expressions meaning the following.

1.  very complicated or detailed;

2.  in succession; one after the other;

3.  having a quality that stimulates interest;

4.  to cause to happen;

5.  the action of putting something in or between;

6.  (especially of a change or distinction) so delicate or precise as to be difficult to analyse or describe;

7.  to make some small changes to something in an attempt to improve it or repair it;

8.  to make someone or something have a quality very strongly;

9.  the ability to be easily modified;

10. finally;

11. to compose;

12. not damaged in any way; complete

Task2. Say whether the following is true, false or not mentioned

1.  If all the conditions are equal, mice grown in the laboratory are absolutely identical and exhibit the same behavior.

2.  The actions we take during our life and our feelings and impressions, all these affect neuron connections in our brain.

3.  There is growing evidence nowadays that other factors should be considered, for example, alternative splicing in which a change in one gene may result in several identical proteins.

4.  Epigenetic changes modify the activation of certain genes, but not the genetic code sequence of DNA.

5.  Jumping genes insert themselves into a human cell resulting in changes in its behavior.

6.  Though the existence of mobile elements has been known for a long time, scientists had no idea their activity would be so intense.

7.  McClintock’s research was immediately recognized by a scientific community and right after its publication she was awarded a Nobel prize.

8.  A good example of reverse transcription is the HIV virus which changes the DNA of the cell it infects.

Task3. Answer the following questions.

1.  What factors affect the human brain causing its diversity?

2.  Why are proteins so important?

3.  What new factor has recently been discovered?

4.  Can we say that there are as many jumping genes in the brain as in the othewer tissues? Why? Why not?

5.  How do jumping genes affect cells?

6.  Why did this process develop in the course of evolution? What’s its meaning?

7.  What was the main idea of McClintock’s experiments?

8.  How did the scientific community react to her discovery?

9.  What’s the difference between her research and more recent research on mobile elements in the human brain?

Part2, p.2- 4 (from ‘Retrotransposition often fails…’ up to ‘ Origins of disease’)      

Task1. Find words/ expressions meaning the following.

1.  it is obvious or logical;

2.  informed about , acquainted with ;

3.  to reproduce or cause to reproduce; breed;

4.  to wait , be in (a state of) readiness ;

5.  to deal with , engage in;

6.  verification , proof;

7.  mixed ,diversified;

8.  to come to light, be known;

9.  subjection , vulnerability ;

10. to consider as possible;

11. initiator

Task2. Say whether the following is true, false or not mentioned.

1.  Truncated, nonfunctional copies of the original L1 DNA have either no effect on a protein-coding gene or affect it badly resulting in a great harm to a cell.

2.  Most scientists are still sure that retransposition takes place mostly in germ cells though there is evidence to the contrary.

3.  Retransposition is more typical of the brain and is a good proof that the genetic codes of brain cells of grownups differ and can change during the cell life.

4.  In future experiments, the researchers will attempt to block retrotransposition at various times in development using RNA interference to look for any changes in cell behavior.

5.  As the research goes on, the brain will be found to be more and more genetically diverse.

6.  The researchers are sure that anew and unknown environment could also result in retransposition.

7.  In a study of rodents neural stem cells, researchers discovered an up to twofold increase in the retrotransposons, which can jump around the genome.

Task3. Answer the following questions.

1.  How do truncated unfunctional copies of the original L1 DNA affect a protein coding gene?

2.  What is the argument against retransposition being confined to germ cells?

3.  What does the fact that retransposition is more frequent in the brain mean?

4.  How was it proved?

5.  Where are nerve cells generated?

6.  Why did human postmortem material have to be examined?

7.  What did the Roslin team discover?

8.  Why were they surprised?

9.  What factors increased gene jumping?

10. Why is this finding so significant?

Part3, p.4-5 (from ‘Origins of disease’ up to the end )

Task1. Find words/ expressions meaning the following

1.  having a harmful or damaging effect ;

2.  to explain;

3.  fragment , trace;

4.  stop oneself from ,refrain;

5.  defect;

6.  differ in size, amount, degree;

7.  unsystematic , unmethodical;

8.  harmful;

9.  improbable in nature; unlikely

10. to make something (e.g. an idea) more believable

Task2. Say whether the following is true, false or not mentioned.

1.The fact that the number of L1 insertions in mice brain Increases greatly due to the mutations in the brain of mice with Rett syndrome means that the jumping genes are surely responsible for some effects of the MeCP2 mutation.

2. The effects of L1 activity are most likely to be neutral or deleterious.

3. Because of the latest findings scientists express doubt about the genetic identity of twins, the differences being due to jumping genes.

4. There are convincing facts which prove that that L1elements are an important factor resulting in various brain disorders.

5. Sometimes the genetic mechanism of L1 retroelements increases our chances of survival which explains why L1s may jump around the genome under carefully controlled conditions.

6. In mice and rats a change in the work of a single neuron is enough to influence their behavior.

7. L1 jumping elements appear to have been a crucial factor in the origin and development of man.

Task3. . Answer the following questions.

1.  How do jumping genes affect people?

2.  Why is it easier to notice unfavourable effects?

3.  What is Rett syndrome and what causes it?

4.  Is it the only disease caused by L1 elements? Why? Why not?

5.  Why is it wrong to consider twins genetically identical?

6.  How does it happen that people still have traces of ancient viruses despite millions of years of evolution?

7.  When did L1 jumping elements originate?

8.  Are they peculiar to humans or can they be found in apes and monkeys, too? (Prove!)

Дата: 2019-02-02, просмотров: 192.